Thalamic control of sensory selection in divided attention (2024)

Table of Contents
References Acknowledgements Author information Authors and Affiliations Contributions Corresponding author Ethics declarations Competing interests Extended data figures and tables Extended Data Figure 1 Cross-modal task training and performance validation. Extended Data Figure 2 Effects of cross-modal divided attention in the mouse. Extended Data Figure 3 Comparable performance on trial types and intact overall auditory performance despite auditory stimuli being eliminated on a subset of ‘attend to vision’ trials. Extended Data Figure 4 Region- and timing-specific effects of optogenetic manipulation on cross-modal task performance. Extended Data Figure 5 Independently adjustable, multi-electrode recording of visTRN neurons. Extended Data Figure 6 Distinct changes in visTRN firing rate during natural errors compared to errors due to PFC disruption. Extended Data Figure 7 The effect of PFC disruption on visTRN activity is distinct from naturally occurring errors. Extended Data Figure 8 The magnitude of behavioural disruption co-varies with the strength of optogenetic manipulation of the LGN or visTRN. Extended Data Figure 9 Attentional modulation by LGN is not observed on error trials. Extended Data Figure 10 Light-evoked fast transients from chloride photometry measured in the LGN are GABAA-receptor dependent and sensitive to visTRN and prelimbic inactivation in the cross-modal task. Supplementary information Supplementary Information Example trials of cross-modal performance PowerPoint slides PowerPoint slide for Fig. 1 PowerPoint slide for Fig. 2 PowerPoint slide for Fig. 3 PowerPoint slide for Fig. 4 PowerPoint slide for Fig. 5 Rights and permissions About this article Cite this article References

References

  1. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202 (2001)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  2. Buschman, T. J. & Miller, E. K. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862 (2007)

    Article CAS ADS Google Scholar

  3. Fritz, J., Shamma, S., Elhilali, M. & Klein, D. Rapid task-related plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Nature Neurosci. 6, 1216–1223 (2003)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  4. Rodgers, C. C. & DeWeese, M. R. Neural correlates of task switching in prefrontal cortex and primary auditory cortex in a novel stimulus selection task for rodents. Neuron 82, 1157–1170 (2014)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  5. Zhang, S. et al. Selective attention. Long-range and local circuits for top-down modulation of visual cortex processing. Science 345, 660–665 (2014)

    Article CAS ADS Google Scholar

  6. Glickfeld, L. L., Histed, M. H. & Maunsell, J. H. Mouse primary visual cortex is used to detect both orientation and contrast changes. J. Neurosci. 33, 19416–19422 (2013)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  7. Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 160, 106–154 (1962)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  8. Newsome, W. T., Britten, K. H. & Movshon, J. A. Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature 341, 52–54 (1989)

    Article CAS ADS Google Scholar

  9. Hoover, W. B. & Vertes, R. P. Anatomical analysis of afferent projections to the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Struct. Funct. 212, 149–179 (2007)

    Article Google Scholar

  10. Vong, L. et al. Leptin action on GABAergic neurons prevents obesity and reduces inhibitory tone to POMC neurons. Neuron 71, 142–154 (2011)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  11. Halassa, M. M. et al. Selective optical drive of thalamic reticular nucleus generates thalamic bursts and cortical spindles. Nature Neurosci. 14, 1118–1120 (2011)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  12. Zhao, S. et al. Cell type–specific channelrhodopsin-2 transgenic mice for optogenetic dissection of neural circuitry function. Nature Methods 8, 745–752 (2011)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  13. Fritz, J. B., David, S. V., Radtke-Schuller, S., Yin, P. & Shamma, S. A. Adaptive, behaviorally gated, persistent encoding of task-relevant auditory information in ferret frontal cortex. Nature Neurosci. 13, 1011–1019 (2010)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  14. Letzkus, J. J. et al. A disinhibitory microcircuit for associative fear learning in the auditory cortex. Nature 480, 331–335 (2011)

    Article CAS ADS Google Scholar

  15. McAlonan, K., Cavanaugh, J. & Wurtz, R. H. Guarding the gateway to cortex with attention in visual thalamus. Nature 456, 391–394 (2008)

    Article CAS ADS Google Scholar

  16. Purushothaman, G., Marion, R., Li, K. & Casagrande, V. A. Gating and control of primary visual cortex by pulvinar. Nature Neurosci. 15, 905–912 (2012)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  17. Saalmann, Y. B., Pinsk, M. A., Wang, L., Li, X. & Kastner, S. The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science 337, 753–756 (2012)

    Article CAS ADS Google Scholar

  18. Pinault, D. The thalamic reticular nucleus: structure, function and concept. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 46, 1–31 (2004)

    Article Google Scholar

  19. Crick, F. Function of the thalamic reticular complex: the searchlight hypothesis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 81, 4586–4590 (1984)

    Article CAS ADS Google Scholar

  20. Halassa, M. M. et al. State-dependent architecture of thalamic reticular subnetworks. Cell 158, 808–821 (2014)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  21. O’Connor, D. H., f*ckui, M. M., Pinsk, M. A. & Kastner, S. Attention modulates responses in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature Neurosci. 5, 1203–1209 (2002)

    Article Google Scholar

  22. Chen, C. & Regehr, W. G. Presynaptic modulation of the retinogeniculate synapse. J. Neurosci. 23, 3130–3135 (2003)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  23. Cox, C. L., Huguenard, J. R. & Prince, D. A. Nucleus reticularis neurons mediate diverse inhibitory effects in thalamus. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 94, 8854–8859 (1997)

    Article CAS ADS Google Scholar

  24. Gunaydin, L. A. et al. Natural neural projection dynamics underlying social behavior. Cell 157, 1535–1551 (2014)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  25. Grimley, J. S. et al. Visualization of synaptic inhibition with an optogenetic sensor developed by cell-free protein engineering automation. J. Neurosci. 33, 16297–16309 (2013)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  26. Cremers, T. & Ebert, B. Plasma and CNS concentrations of Gaboxadol in rats following subcutaneous administration. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 562, 47–52 (2007)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  27. Casagrande, V. A., Sáry, G., Royal, D. & Ruiz, O. On the impact of attention and motor planning on the lateral geniculate nucleus. Prog. Brain Res. 149, 11–29 (2005)

    Article Google Scholar

  28. Mitchell, A. S. et al. Advances in understanding mechanisms of thalamic relays in cognition and behavior. J. Neurosci. 34, 15340–15346 (2014)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  29. Ress, D. & Heeger, D. J. Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex. Nature Neurosci. 6, 414–420 (2003)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  30. Levitan, C. A., Ban, Y. H., Stiles, N. R. & Shimojo, S. Rate perception adapts across the senses: evidence for a unified timing mechanism. Sci. Rep. 5, 8857 (2015)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  31. Mareschal, I., Calder, A. J., Dadds, M. R. & Clifford, C. W. Gaze categorization under uncertainty: psychophysics and modeling. J. Vis. 13, 18 (2013)

    Article Google Scholar

  32. Hastie, T., Tibshirani, R. & Friedman, J. The Elements of Statistical Learning: Data Mining, Inference and Prediction 2nd edn, Ch. 7 (Springer, 2009)

    Book Google Scholar

  33. Wallace, D. J. et al. Rats maintain an overhead binocular field at the expense of constant fusion. Nature 498, 65–69 (2013)

    Article CAS ADS Google Scholar

  34. Brunetti, P. M. et al. Design and fabrication of ultralight weight, adjustable multi-electrode probes for electrophysiological recordings in mice. J. Vis. Exp. 91, e51675 (2014)

    Google Scholar

  35. Fries, P., Neuenschwander, S., Engel, A. K., Goebel, R. & Singer, W. Rapid feature selective neuronal synchronization through correlated latency shifting. Nature Neurosci. 4, 194–200 (2001)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  36. Szucs, A. Applications of the spike density function in analysis of neuronal firing patterns. J. Neurosci. Methods 81, 159–167 (1998)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  37. Piscopo, D. M., El-Danaf, R. N., Huberman, A. D. & Niell, C. M. Diverse visual features encoded in mouse lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Neurosci. 33, 4642–4656 (2013)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  38. De Araujo, I. E. et al. Neural ensemble coding of satiety states. Neuron 51, 483–494 (2006)

    Article CAS Google Scholar

  39. Ridder III, W. H. & Nusinowitz, S. The visual evoked potential in the mouse–origins and response characteristics. Vision Res. 46, 902–913 (2006)

    Article Google Scholar

  40. Izaki, Y., Fujiwara, S. E. & Akema, T. Rat prefrontal response and prestimulation local field potential power in vivo. Neuroreport 19, 255–258 (2008)

    Article Google Scholar

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank J. A. Movshon, W. Ma, R. W. Tsien, G. Fishell and D. Rinberg for helpful comments on the manuscript and G. J. Augustine for providing us with the SuperClomeleon construct and for helpful discussion around its use. The work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (P2LAP3 151786) to R.D.W. and the Simons Foundation, the Sloan Foundation, the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation and the US National Institutes of Health (R00 NS078115) to M.M.H; M.M.H. is additionally supported by the Feldstein Medical Foundation, a Klingenstein-Simons Fellowship and a Biobehavioral Research Award for Innovative New Scientists (BRAINS) R01 (R01 MH107680) from the National Institute of Mental Health.

Author information

Author notes

  1. Ralf D. Wimmer and L. Ian Schmitt: These authors contributed equally to this work.

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Department of Neuroscience and Physiology, New York University Neuroscience Institute, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, 10016, New York, USA

    Ralf D. Wimmer,L. Ian Schmitt,Miho Nakajima&Michael M. Halassa

  2. Department of Bioengineering, Stanford University, Stanford, 94305, California, USA

    Thomas J. Davidson&Karl Deisseroth

  3. Cracking the Neural Code Program, Stanford University, Stanford, 94305, California, USA

    Karl Deisseroth

  4. Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, 94305, California, USA

    Karl Deisseroth

  5. Department of Psychiatry, New York University Langone Medical Center, New York, 10016, New York, USA

    Michael M. Halassa

  6. Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, 10003, New York, USA

    Michael M. Halassa

Authors

  1. Ralf D. Wimmer

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  2. L. Ian Schmitt

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  3. Thomas J. Davidson

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  4. Miho Nakajima

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  5. Karl Deisseroth

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

  6. Michael M. Halassa

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMedGoogle Scholar

Contributions

M.M.H. conceived and designed all aspects of the study. R.D.W. devised the training paradigm for the cross-modal task and L.I.S. performed all associated programming. R.D.W. collected electrophysiological data. T.J.D. provided fibre photometry training, advice and rig designs; L.I.S. extended the method to FRET-based photometry, built the rig and collected data. R.D.W. analysed behavioural data and L.I.S. analysed psychophysical, electrophysiological and photometry data. M.N. generated the retrograde lentiviruses in-house, performed SuperClomeleon cloning into an AAV backbone and acquired confocal images. K.D. provided support for fibre photometry training. M.M.H. supervised the experiment, directed the analysis and wrote the manuscript. All authors read the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael M. Halassa.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Extended data figures and tables

Extended Data Figure 1 Cross-modal task training and performance validation.

Quantification of performance across training stages for the cross-modal task. The trial sequence for each training stage is indicated on the left. Improved performance was observed in the last three days of training relative to the first three for each stage. Bar graphs on the left (column 1) show the reduction in the error fraction (n = 15 mice,*P < 0.05, Wilcoxon rank-sum test), column 2 shows the number of consecutive correct responses (P-values shown, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test) and bar graphs on the right (column 3) show the probability of a correct response following a modality shift (*P < 0.05, Wilcoxon rank-sum test).

Extended Data Figure 2 Effects of cross-modal divided attention in the mouse.

Top row, single-mouse examples of visual detection performance during cross-modal divided attention and reversal learning. Comparison of performance under visual-only (black) and cross-modal (green) conditions are shown on the left. Although neither condition contained sensory conflict, the mere expectation of one increased detection threshold (≥124 trials per condition). Detection threshold was not affected by the presence of an auditory distractor during reversal learning (≥90 trials per condition), as shown on the right. Middle and bottom rows, group data normalized to peak performance (lapse rate), showing that the effects of divided attention on detection threshold were persistent. Bootstrap estimation of visual detection threshold shows a similar pattern as data in Fig. 1 (error bars are 95% confidence intervals).

Extended Data Figure 3 Comparable performance on trial types and intact overall auditory performance despite auditory stimuli being eliminated on a subset of ‘attend to vision’ trials.

Left, performance was comparable on auditory and maximum-intensity visual trials (n = 4 mice, same as in Fig. 1d). Right, mice exhibited comparable overall performance when auditory stimuli were eliminated from a subset of ‘attend to vision’ trials.

Extended Data Figure 4 Region- and timing-specific effects of optogenetic manipulation on cross-modal task performance.

a, Optogenetic disruption of auditory cortex during target stimulus anticipation disrupted performance specifically for auditory trials (n = 4 mice, **P < 0.01, Wilcoxon rank-sum test). Disruption of AAC (b) or lateral OFC (c) in VGAT-ChR2 mice or following localized injection of a ChR2-expressing virus did not affect performance (n = 4 mice (2 VGAT-ChR2 and 2 VGAT-Cre), 4 sessions per manipulation). d, In contrast, inactivation of prelimbic (PL) cortex led to robust reduction in performance in both types of manipulation (n = 8 mice (4 VGAT-ChR2 and 4 VGAT-Cre), *P < 0.05 Wilcoxon rank-sum test). eh, Photobleaching experiment to quantify the spread of laser light. A coronal section (e) shows GFP bleaching following two-hour exposure to laser stimulation (6 mW, 50 Hz, 90% duty cycle). fh, Fluorescence intensity quantification shows that the extent of light spread is limited to 300 μm around the tip of the optic fibre (n = 3 mice). Ant, anterior; FrA, frontal association cortex; lat, lateral; LO, lateral orbitofrontal cortex; med, medial; MO, medial orbitofrontal cortex; post, posterior; Stim, stimulation; Sham, sham surgery control; VO, ventral orbitofrontal cortex. Scale bar in e, 200 µm.

Extended Data Figure 5 Independently adjustable, multi-electrode recording of visTRN neurons.

a, b, Injection of DIO-ChR2-eYFP retrograde lentivirus into LGN labels visTRN neurons but not LGN interneurons. a, The histological image is the maximal projection of four 2-μm confocal planes showing labelling of visTRN neurons (inset shows a zoom view of cell bodies) approximately 1.34 mm posterior to Bregma. b, Image as in a, but from LGN of the same animal, approximately 2.46 mm posterior to Bregma (inset shows a zoom view of terminals). c, Schematic of independently adjustable multi-electrode drive. d, An example of activity recorded from different depths during adjustment. Distinct patterns of physiological activity are observed along the trajectory in the broadband local field potential signal (0.1 Hz–32 kHz). The numbers correspond to different recording sites (marked by the red dots on theschematic in c). e, High-pass-filtered signals (600 Hz–10 kHz) showing spiking activity, with isolated clustered units showing distinguishable waveform characteristics in distinct structures. f, Example peri-event time histograms of ChR2-mediated visTRN response. Top, response to laser activation (473 nm, ˜4 mW, stimulation, 20 ms). Bottom, response to visual stimuli (10-ms pulse). Blue and orange blocks indicate laser and visual stimulation, respectively.

Extended Data Figure 6 Distinct changes in visTRN firing rate during natural errors compared to errors due to PFC disruption.

ac, Scatter plots showing the change in absolute firing rate for visTRN neurons for correct (a), incorrect (b) or disrupted-PFC trials (c). Insets show the cumulative probability plot of separation from the unity line (no change). Although correct trials had a lower firing rate in ‘attend to vision’ than in ‘attend to audition’ trials (n = 138, P < 0.001 Wilcoxon signed-rank test), this pattern was reversed for incorrect trials (n = 138, P < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed-rank test); this suggests that perhaps the animal was attending to the wrong modality. This reversal was not observed in trials with PFC disruption (despite mouse performance being at chance level).

Extended Data Figure 7 The effect of PFC disruption on visTRN activity is distinct from naturally occurring errors.

a, Scatter plots of response from visTRN neurons, comparing the modulation of their firing rate (change from baseline) under the two distinct anticipatory conditions. Each sample is a single cell. Colours denote significance reached for each cell on a trial-by-trial basis (red, visual; blue, auditory; purple, both; rank-sum-test comparison to baseline). Note that in correct performance (n = 138, 4 mice, P < 0.005, Wilcoxon signed-rank test), ‘attend to vision’ resulted in a negative shift and ‘attend to audition’ resulted in a positive shift, consistent with examples shown in Fig. 3. During naturally occurring error trials, the modulation is partially reversed for both trial types, suggesting that at least a subset of errors are the result of attending to the wrong modality. In contrast, PFC disruption (n = 56 cells, 2 mice) resulted in a weaker, non-uniform effect (‘attend to visual’ trials are less affected). b, Quantification of effects seen in a. N.S., not significant.

Extended Data Figure 8 The magnitude of behavioural disruption co-varies with the strength of optogenetic manipulation of the LGN or visTRN.

Activation of inhibitory terminals in the LGN with a 90% duty cycle laser (Fig. 2) resulted in maximal disruption of cross-modal performance. Activating visually labelled TRN with identical stimulation parameters resulted in a quantitatively lower behavioural effect. Reducing the duty cycle of visTRN stimulation to 10% resulted in no effect on accuracy, as shown previously4.

Extended Data Figure 9 Attentional modulation by LGN is not observed on error trials.

a, b, No significant difference was observed in the average firing rate of LGN neurons during stimulus anticipation (P = 0.63, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, n = 161 cells, 4 mice) or presentation (P = 0.74, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, n = 161) among trial types when behavioural outcomes were incorrect. c, Similar effects were observed for VEPs (visual, n = 324 trials; auditory, n = 302 trials; 4 mice).

Extended Data Figure 10 Light-evoked fast transients from chloride photometry measured in the LGN are GABAA-receptor dependent and sensitive to visTRN and prelimbic inactivation in the cross-modal task.

a, Peak SuperClomeleon FRET- and YFP-control responses to light stimuli (50 ms, 0.1 Hz) delivered to the eye contralateral to the recorded LGN (n > 90 trials from 3 mice for SuperClomeleon and from 4 mice for YFP, ***P < 0.001, Friedman test). b, Chloride photometry transients are sensitive to the GABAA receptor antagonist flumazenil in a dose-dependent manner. Left, intraperitoneal injection of 15 mg kg–1 flumazenil resulted in a 90% peak reduction of light-evoked chloride photometry responses, which recovered over the course of 90–100 min as predicted by flumazenil pharmaco*kinetics. Insets show example traces of single events recorded during baseline, peak suppression and recovery. Right, quantification of the maximal suppressive effects and recovery of 5 mg kg–1 and 15 mg kg–1 flumazenil on chloride photometry responses (n> 90 trials from 3 mice, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, Friedman test). c, Cumulative distributions of unitary visual-evoked SuperClomeleon FRET peaks in response to light stimuli in the cross-modal task. Under baseline conditions, ‘attend to audition’ trials exhibited significantly larger amplitudes than ‘attend to vision’ trials, consistent with average data in Fig. 5f. Optogenetic silencing of visTRN neurons eliminated the difference between trial types and resulted in peak amplitudes comparable to baseline ‘attend to vision’ trials (n = 3 mice, P < 0.005 for ‘attend to audition’ trials vs all other trial types, Kolmogorov–Smirnov statistics with Bonferroni correction). d, Combined optogenetic and chloride photometry inactivation of different frontal cortical regions in the LGN while mice performed the cross-modal task. Only PL inactivation eliminates differential inhibition between visual and auditory trials (n = 6 mice, ***P < 0.001, Wilcoxon rank-sum test).

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

This file contains a Supplementary Discussion and additional references. (PDF 322 kb)

Example trials of cross-modal performance

Three trials are shown; the first is ‘attend to vision’ (left selection) the second is ‘attend to audition’ (left selection), and the third is ‘attend to vision’ (right selection). All trials are shown in normal speed and in slow-motion. The video illustrates the mechanics of the task and the impact of context (cueing) on selection. (MP4 26582 kb)

Rights and permissions

About this article

Thalamic control of sensory selection in divided attention (1)

Cite this article

Wimmer, R., Schmitt, L., Davidson, T. et al. Thalamic control of sensory selection in divided attention. Nature 526, 705–709 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature15398

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature15398

Thalamic control of sensory selection in divided attention (2024)

References

Top Articles
ADC MANAGEMENT SERVICES, INC. hiring General Clerk RCTG CO Missoula (Butte) in Missoula, Montana, United States | LinkedIn
State to issue reappraisals to Browning next week, leaders want answers
Windcrest Little League Baseball
Western Union Mexico Rate
Seething Storm 5E
Arrests reported by Yuba County Sheriff
The Haunted Drury Hotels of San Antonio’s Riverwalk
Mikayla Campinos Videos: A Deep Dive Into The Rising Star
New Mexico Craigslist Cars And Trucks - By Owner
Aces Fmc Charting
Gfs Rivergate
What Happened To Maxwell Laughlin
Shannon Dacombe
Price Of Gas At Sam's
Moviesda3.Com
Me Cojo A Mama Borracha
Sound Of Freedom Showtimes Near Cinelux Almaden Cafe & Lounge
Vipleaguenba
Loves Employee Pay Stub
Hennens Chattanooga Dress Code
Danforth's Port Jefferson
Www.publicsurplus.com Motor Pool
Pecos Valley Sunland Park Menu
Wemod Vampire Survivors
Used Safari Condo Alto R1723 For Sale
Where to eat: the 50 best restaurants in Freiburg im Breisgau
Crossword Help - Find Missing Letters & Solve Clues
How To Find Free Stuff On Craigslist San Diego | Tips, Popular Items, Safety Precautions | RoamBliss
D2L Brightspace Clc
Craigslist Texas Killeen
Busted! 29 New Arrests in Portsmouth, Ohio – 03/27/22 Scioto County Mugshots
Chapaeva Age
Ixlggusd
Max 80 Orl
Pensacola 311 Citizen Support | City of Pensacola, Florida Official Website
Msnl Seeds
Chuze Fitness La Verne Reviews
Henry County Illuminate
Umd Men's Basketball Duluth
Leland Nc Craigslist
2017 Ford F550 Rear Axle Nut Torque Spec
Bustednewspaper.com Rockbridge County Va
Walmart 24 Hrs Pharmacy
Playboi Carti Heardle
Server Jobs Near
Turok: Dinosaur Hunter
Used Sawmill For Sale - Craigslist Near Tennessee
The Latest Books, Reports, Videos, and Audiobooks - O'Reilly Media
Urban Airship Acquires Accengage, Extending Its Worldwide Leadership With Unmatched Presence Across Europe
2487872771
Bluebird Valuation Appraiser Login
Obituary Roger Schaefer Update 2020
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Ray Christiansen

Last Updated:

Views: 6323

Rating: 4.9 / 5 (69 voted)

Reviews: 84% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Ray Christiansen

Birthday: 1998-05-04

Address: Apt. 814 34339 Sauer Islands, Hirtheville, GA 02446-8771

Phone: +337636892828

Job: Lead Hospitality Designer

Hobby: Urban exploration, Tai chi, Lockpicking, Fashion, Gunsmithing, Pottery, Geocaching

Introduction: My name is Ray Christiansen, I am a fair, good, cute, gentle, vast, glamorous, excited person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.